, 2009 and Tkach et al , 2007) Monkeys were trained to perform p

, 2009 and Tkach et al., 2007). Monkeys were trained to perform planar movements in order to move a cursor to randomly positioned targets presented visually on a horizontal screen. During active performance the cursor’s position was dictated by the endpoint of a two-link exoskeletal robot moved by the monkey’s arm. The cursor movements and accompanying targets were recorded and subsequently replayed to the animal during the observation phase providing the same visual stimulation as active performance. We required the monkeys to voluntarily maintain the position of

their arm in a fixed posture during observation Lapatinib mouse to eliminate the effects of arm movements on the neural activity measured in MI. We found a tremendous amount of heterogeneity when examining the responses of individual neurons during active performance and observation. The firing rate profiles of some neurons exhibited noticeable differences under the two conditions (Figure 3A) while others modulated similarly in the two tasks (Figure 3B). Most surprising was a group of cells whose firing rate

was modulated during action observation but did not discharge in response to active movement of the limb (Figure 3C). While some neurons exhibited significant mutual information between spiking activity and cursor direction during active performance but not during observation (Figure 3D), a number of neurons showed significant mutual information during observation XAV-939 ic50 (Figure 3E), these and, in some cases, there was significant mutual information only during observation (Figure 3F). This analysis provided evidence that MI may be a member of this putative “mirror” neuron system. Mutual information estimates indicated that neural modulation led cursor movement for both voluntarily executed movement and passively observed cursor movement. This prospective activity during observation in MI is consistent

with a role in the mental rehearsal of action and is similar to other reports in both PMd (Cisek and Kalaska, 2004) and MI (Dushanova and Donoghue, 2010). However, the time lag between neural activity and movement tended to be shorter for observed cursor movements (approximately 50 ms, e.g., Figures 3E and 3F) compared to overt arm movements (approximately 125 ms, e.g., Figure 3D) meaning that the neural modulation occurred closer in time to the movement itself. This decrease in lag time may reflect the change in the dynamic properties of the task between active performance and observation. That is, during active performance the motor commands issued by MI are filtered by the motor plant (e.g., transmission, muscle recruitment, and inertial delays) causing the typical delay between activity in cortex and subsequent behavior.

If we define a region of interest (ROI) based on previous imaging

If we define a region of interest (ROI) based on previous imaging studies implicating the right TPJ (centered at [x, y, z] = [54, −54, 24], see Experimental Procedures) and compute the correlation between GM volume and β, we also obtain a high and significant correlation (Figure 3B, r = 0.61, p < 0.001), while preferences for altruism in the domain of disadvantageous inequality α are

uncorrelated with GM volume (Figure 3C, r = −0.01, p = 0.95). These results suggest a specific role of the TPJ in altruistic behaviors Screening Library screening in the domain of advantageous inequality. In addition to measuring the baseline levels of altruistic preferences in the domain of advantageous and disadvantageous inequality, our behavioral experiments also enable us to measure preferences for positive and negative reciprocity (Supplemental Information). Based on models of reciprocity developed in economics (Dufwenberg and Kirchsteiger, 2004,

Falk and Fischbacher, 2006 and Rabin, 1993), we define positive reciprocity as the motive to respond in a kind manner Linsitinib to acts that are perceived as kind. In contrast, negative reciprocity is defined as the motive to respond in a hostile manner to acts that are perceived as hostile. According to this notion of reciprocity, individuals who are motivated by reciprocity are willing to behave reciprocally even if the reciprocal act is associated with a net cost for the acting party, i.e., even if there are no future material benefits that outweigh the cost of the reciprocal action. Thus, positive reciprocity means that a subject responds altruistically (i.e., increases the partner’s payoff at his own cost) to an action of the partner that is perceived as kind relative to a neutral action; negative reciprocity means that a subject decreases a partner’s payoff

at his own cost in response to an action that is perceived to be hostile relative to a neutral action. We embed the notion of intention-based reciprocity into our model of social preferences that is based on Charness and Rabin (2002) and Fehr and Schmidt (1999). In our extended model, we measure an individual’s preferences for positive reciprocity with parameter θ, while parameter δ represents preferences for negative reciprocity. Interestingly, neither θ nor δ is significantly correlated with TPJ GM volume (Figures 3D and 3E) or found with any other brain region (Table S2), which further supports the specificity of our finding for baseline altruism in the domain of advantageous inequality. We also conducted a multiple regression analysis to examine the robustness of the association between TPJ GM volume and β while controlling for all other preference parameters (α, δ, θ), as well as for age, gender, political attitude, and autistic traits. Again, β is highly significant (p = 0.004, Table S3), while no other preference parameters are significantly correlated (all p > 0.5) with TPJ GM volume.

In addition there are delays on the efferent control signals, bot

In addition there are delays on the efferent control signals, both in terms of neural conduction delays and the low-pass properties of muscle. Although the fastest conduction delays, such as the monosynaptic stretch reflex pathway, are on the order of 10–40 ms, depending on the length and type

of nerve fiber, this delay increases by 20–30 ms for the cortical component of the long-latency stretch reflex response (Matthews, 1991). Selleckchem Ku0059436 Moreover, the rise in the force generation within a muscle (termed the electromechanical delay) can take another 25 ms (Ito et al., 2004). This means that a descending command from the motor cortex takes around 40 ms to produce force in the muscle because the conduction delay from the motor cortex to the arm muscles is around 16 ms (Merton and Morton, 1980). Other modalities Z-VAD-FMK ic50 can take even longer, with the delay in involuntary motor responses due to visual stimuli of around 110–150 ms (Day and Lyon, 2000, Franklin and Wolpert, 2008 and Saijo et al., 2005). Even the vestibulo-ocular reflex, one of the fastest involuntary responses due to the short connections, takes 10 ms from stimulus onset (Aw et al., 2006). At one extreme, such as a saccadic eye movement, the movement duration is shorter than the sensory delay, meaning that feedback cannot

be used to guide the movement because the sensory information regarding the movement itself arrives after the completion of the movement. For slower movements, delays make control difficult because information can

be out of date, and it is possible ADP ribosylation factor for the system to correct for errors that no longer exist, leading to potential instability. Uncertainty reflects incomplete knowledge either with regard to the state of the world or of the task or rewards we might receive. Although uncertainty about the present state can arise from both noise and delays, there are many other sources of uncertainty; for example, it can arise from the limitations in receptor density and the representation of an analog world with the digital neural code. Uncertainty can also arise from the inherent ambiguity in sensory processing, such as ambiguity that arises when the three-dimensional world is projected onto the two-dimensional retina (Yuille and Kersten, 2006). Other components of uncertainty arise from the inherent ambiguity of the world. When we first see or even handle a new object, we may be unsure of its properties such as its dynamics. Similarly, when we first experience a novel environment, such as forces applied to the arm during a reaching movement (Shadmehr and Mussa-Ivaldi, 1994), we only receive partial information about the environmental properties even if we had perfect sensory information. Other situations, such as those that are unstable (Burdet et al.

, 2012), serving as proof-of-concept that apoE4 is a promising ta

, 2012), serving as proof-of-concept that apoE4 is a promising target for the development of small molecule–based therapeutics. Blocking domain interaction in apoE4 reverses many of its detrimental effects, both in vitro and in vivo (Mahley and Huang, 2012). This can be accomplished by site-directed mutagenesis in which arginine-61 is exchanged for threonine, thereby preventing the ionic interaction, or

by small-molecule structure correctors that interact with apoE in the vicinity of arginine-61 to prevent or retard domain interaction. Importantly, blocking Vorinostat in vitro domain interaction by site-directed mutagenesis or small-molecule structure correctors markedly reduced proteolysis and fragment formation. Mitochondrial dysfunction was no longer observed in cells expressing an apoE4 variant that lacked the ability to undergo domain interaction (apoE4-R61T). Furthermore, a small-molecule structure corrector restored the level of complex IV mitochondrial cytochrome c oxidase in apoE4-expressing cells to levels seen in apoE3-expressing cells (Figure 9C; Chen et al., 2012). These studies were expanded to identify potent apoE4 structure correctors that could restore the level of mitochondrial cytochrome c oxidase with the potential to be used in vivo. A class of such small-molecule Enzalutamide molecular weight compounds

that displays a significant structure-activity relationship ADP ribosylation factor has been identified (Chen et al., 2012). As described, blocking apoE4 domain interaction restores neurite outgrowth, mitochondrial motility, and synaptic density (Brodbeck et al., 2011; Chen et al., 2011a). Thus, apoE4 domain interaction is a critical structural element that modulates both the physiological and pathophysiological functions of apoE4 (Mahley and Huang, 2012). The studies reviewed here, which

comprise only a subset of the work done on apoE4 in the central nervous system, overwhelmingly point to a critical direct role for apoE4 in AD-mediated neurodegeneration. Based upon these studies, we propose the following model (Figure 10) to illustrate this hypothesis. Figure 10 (1): What is well established is that neuronal injury or stress, caused by a variety of injurious agents, induces the synthesis of apoE by neurons. The structural properties of each apoE isoform dictate its propensity to undergo domain interaction (apoE4 > apoE3 > apoE2), which leads to apoE isoform-dependent proteolysis and the generation of neurotoxic fragments. In turn, these fragments cause mitochondrial dysfunction and cytoskeletal alterations, leading to neurodegeneration (Huang, 2010; Huang and Mucke, 2012; Mahley et al., 2006). Although much remains to be understood about how apoE function affects both health and disease states, it is clear that apoE plays a critical role in the pathogenesis of many different neurodegenerative diseases.

There was a statistical difference in right side handgrip strengt

There was a statistical difference in right side handgrip strength when compared painful with painless wrists (p = 0.02). In a 10-gymnasts group, we found 15 wrists presenting pain (5 gymnasts presented both wrists with pain). Nine of these 15 wrists were classified as unrestricted (grade 1) and five wrists were classified as grade 2, when gymnasts could attend all

training sessions but were unable to do full workout. Only one wrist was identified as grade 3 implying this website that this gymnast was forced to miss one training session. Pommel horse was the apparatus most frequently associated with painful wrists (8 out of 15, 53.3%) referred by gymnasts. Concerning the maturity status, most gymnasts were classified on time or average, which is in accordance with previous data on male gymnasts as demonstrated by Baxter–Jones et al.35 and Malina et al.27 UV of immature reference populations is MLN0128 mouse on average negative as demonstrated by the data of Hafner and coworkers.31 Our sample of Portuguese gymnasts showed also, on average, a negative UV. Despite a more negative UV than the normative values from the immature population31 significant differences in relation to the general population could only be found for DIDI-R (p < 0.01). The normative values presented by Hafner et al. 31 in this age group range from −2.2 to −2.3 mm, whereby the results of PRPR (left and right) and DIDI-L from the 23 Portuguese male gymnasts

(7–16 years) did not show significant differences when compared to the general population (ranging from p = 0.55 to p = 0.65). The reason why we decided to use immature reference values from Hafner et al. 31 such as other authors 5, 12 and 16 Phosphatidylinositol diacylglycerol-lyase has to do with is the fact that there are still no reference values for the Portuguese population. While Chang et al.18 did not find significant differences in UV values between their sample and a control group of Chinese musicians, other studies involving gymnasts5, 12 and 36 showed significantly less negative UV when compared with normative values from Hafner et al.,31

which can be justified by the different conditions of the referred studies such as the different methods used to measure UV (perpendicular and Hafner’s methods), different observers, possible differences in laterality and dominance hands, and ethnographic-related factors.37 Ethnographic-related factors can, eventually, explain some UV differences38 and 39 since more positive UV values were found in Black race when compared to Caucasians.38 Additionally Koreans also showed significantly higher UV when compared to Japanese or Chinese subjects.39 The length of ulna relative to the length of the radius is not constant but varies in the course of life.40 Change in UV can be attributed simply to CA, SA, SA–CA, and, in the case of gymnasts, may also be eventually due to training characteristics. In the study of Hafner et al.

Moreover, consistent with the finding that blocking DNA methylati

Moreover, consistent with the finding that blocking DNA methylation in the anterior cingulate cortex prevents remote memory maintenance, another study reported long-lasting changes in methylation of the memory suppressor gene calcineurin within this brain area following contextual fear conditioning ( Miller et al., 2010). These changes in calcineurin methylation persisted at least 30 days following conditioning, suggesting the change is stable enough to maintain a memory over time despite ongoing cellular activity and molecular turnover. Thus, calcineurin

is an excellent candidate for a molecular storage device. Likewise, although they are too numerous to name here, histone modifications have been repeatedly associated with changes in gene transcription and expression in multiple organisms, systems, and brain subregions ( Brami-Cherrier et al., 2005, Dulac, 2010, Guan et al., Trichostatin A molecular weight 2002, Gupta et al., 2010, Koshibu et al., 2009 and Renthal and Nestler, 2008). Thus, these results reveal that even within nondividing neurons in the adult CNS, epigenetic mechanisms regulate patterns of gene expression in a functionally relevant manner. Indeed, when viewed through this lens, epigenetic changes can MDV3100 datasheet simply be viewed as one of the final steps (or perhaps the final step) in a long cascade of events that leads to learning-related

gene transcription ( Kornhauser et al., 2002, Shaywitz GPX6 and Greenberg, 1999 and Sweatt, 2001). A related means for epigenetic control of gene expression involves the unique regulation of specific protein isoforms, or differently spliced versions of the same protein. This can occur in multiple ways, such as increased expression of one exon over another competing exon or silencing of an entire exon. By regulating the expression of splice variants with different cellular functions or different affinities for effector proteins, the potential uses of the same gene locus can be expanded in a multiplicative fashion (Nilsen and Graveley, 2010). The mechanisms that regulate alternative splicing are currently unclear. However, histone modifications appear to modulate this

process by recruiting different splicing regulators that determine splicing outcome (Luco et al., 2010). DNA methylation is also likely involved in the differential expression of BDNF exons following fear learning ( Lubin et al., 2008). Contextual fear conditioning produces a rapid increase in mRNA for BDNF exon IV, thereby decreasing methylation at this locus in area CA1 of the hippocampus. Interestingly, context exposure alone (no conditioning) produced increases in BDNF exon I and VI mRNA, which also corresponded to decreased CpG methylation at these sites. Moreover, intrahippocampal infusions of the DNMT inhibitors zebularine or RG108 impaired fear memory expression, despite the fact that they increase expression of all BDNF exons in naive animals.

e , they

had a learning rate near 1 0) Thus, while the d

e., they

had a learning rate near 1.0). Thus, while the dynamic contingencies strongly induced uncertainty about the value of unexplored options, this manipulation may have paradoxically precluded the Gefitinib cost identification of an uncertainty bonus, because participants believed that only the previous trial was relevant. Frank et al. (2009) recently showed evidence that quantitative trial-by-trial exploratory responses are in part driven by relative uncertainty when reinforcement contingencies are stationary over time. Moreover, substantial individual differences in uncertainty-driven exploration were observed, a large part of which were accounted for by a polymorphism in the catechol-O-methyl transferase (COMT) gene that affects PFC dopamine levels. A subsequent study with the same task found that uncertainty-driven exploration was substantially reduced in patients with schizophrenia as a function of anhedonia, also thought to be related to PFC dysfunction ( Strauss Compound C order et al., 2011). These findings provide a general link between relative uncertainty-based exploration and PFC function. Frank et al. (2009) further hypothesized that RLPFC, in particular, may track relative uncertainty among options. Despite the failure to observe uncertainty-based modulation

of RLPFC activity in previous gambling tasks, the hypothesis that RLPFC computes relative uncertainty is consistent with the broader human neuroimaging literature. Activation in RLPFC is greater during computations of uncertainty during goal attainment in navigation (Yoshida and Ishii, 2006) and has been shown to track relative reward probabilities for alternative courses of action (Boorman et al., 2009). More broadly, growing evidence suggests that RLPFC is at the apex of a caudal to rostral hierarchical organization in frontal cortex (Badre, 2008, Koechlin et al., 2003 and Koechlin and Summerfield, 2007). In this organization, more rostral PFC regions exert control over action at more abstract levels. One conception of abstraction is that which involves tracking

higher-order relations (Braver and Bongiolatti, 2002, Bunge and Wendelken, 2009, Bunge et al., 2005, Christoff et al., 2001, Kroger et al., 2002 and Koechlin et al., 1999). In this respect, Bunge and 3-mercaptopyruvate sulfurtransferase Wendelken (2009) interpreted the Boorman et al. (2009) result as indicative of a more fundamental computation of the RLPFC in tracking the relative advantage of switching to alternative courses of action, rather than of reward probabilities, per se. In keeping with this suggestion, we hypothesized that, while in environments in which participants explore based on relative uncertainty, activation in RLPFC would track changes in relative uncertainty. We further posited that individual differences in uncertainty-driven exploration might be accompanied by differences in the RLPFC response to relative uncertainty.

, 2008) In FXS, the absence of FMRP creates a perpetual translat

, 2008). In FXS, the absence of FMRP creates a perpetual translation “ON” state that leads to increased protein expression of FMRP targets, which not only include key mediators of synaptic plasticity such as PSD-95, CaMKIIα, and Shank3, but also regulators of mTORC1 signaling such as PIKE, OSI-906 datasheet TSC2, Raptor, eIF4G, and eEF2 (indicated

with asterisk in Figure 8A). Darnell et al. (2011) also identified regulators of ERK signaling that are FMRP targets, which, if overexpressed, also would enhance ERK signaling. Enhanced expression of regulators of mTORC1 and ERK would form a feed-forward loop that again would promote general translation. Removal of S6K1 acts as a tonic brake on the exaggerated protein synthesis in FXS by normalizing the phosphorylation and/or expression levels of key translation control molecules such as ribosomal protein S6, eIF4B, and eEF2 and/or by depleting the levels of critical initiation see more factors such as eIF4G ( Figure 8C). This model is consistent with the lowered levels of protein synthesis that occur with deletion of S6K1 as shown by our SUnSET experiments ( Figures 1C and 1D). The experiments we conducted examining FMRP target proteins support this model ( Figures 2A and 2B); however there were some exceptions, notably PSD-95 ( Figures 2A and 2B). Future studies should help

to clarify the role of S6K1 in translational control and to identify classes of FMRP target mRNAs that have multiple, redundant strategies to ensure their translation. S6K1 KO mice are viable, so in the absence of S6K1, protein synthesis still occurs via other modes of

translational control ( Richter and Klann, 2009) and/or the compensatory to actions by other related kinases such as S6K2 and RSK. Consistent with the latter possibility, S6K1 KO mice exhibit increased expression of S6K2 ( Shima et al., 1998). In summary, our findings indicate that the removal of S6K1 corrects multiple pathophysiologies and behavioral abnormalities in FXS model mice. Moreover, the genetic reduction of S6K1 can prevent a broad range of phenotypes, including peripheral abnormalities, associated with FXS that has not been achieved with previous genetic manipulations. With the recent identification of specific inhibitors of S6K1 ( Pearce et al., 2010), we visualize opportunities for translating the results from our genetic experiments to a viable pharmacological approach to target S6K1 to reverse a broad range of phenotypes in FXS model mice. Such an approach may develop into a therapeutic option for humans with FXS in the future. Fmr1 KO/S6K1 KO (dKO) mice were initially generated by crossing heterozygous female mice carrying the Fmr1 mutation with heterozygous male mice carrying the S6K1 mutation. Subsequently, all animals used for experimentation were derived from the crossing of female XFmr1+XFmr1-/ S6K+/− with males either XFmr1+Y/S6K1+/− or XFmr1-Y/S6K1+/−. See the Supplemental Experimental Procedures for detailed information.

, 2009; Lesort et al , 2000) Functional consequences of these in

, 2009; Lesort et al., 2000). Functional consequences of these increases may be complex. For example, exogenous polyamines and transglutaminase can be either neuroprotective or neurotoxic

depending on dose and context. Similarly, changes in MT stability may be AG-014699 solubility dmso beneficial or detrimental depending on level and circumstance. Stable MTs correlate positively with neuronal stability but negatively with neuronal plasticity. The stable-MT fraction is modest but crucial in early development, facilitating axon growth and plasticity. As neurons mature, MT stability increases and neuronal plasticity decreases. As a consequence, neuronal connectivity may be stabilized, maintaining neuronal architecture, but continued declines in plasticity in aging may limit axonal recovery following injury or in neurodegenerative diseases by limiting MT dynamics. LBH589 price For example, inhibiting transglutaminase in models of HD resulted in a modest improvement of lifespan and behavior in HD mouse models. Although transglutaminase may not be a direct component of molecular pathogenesis in HD, it may compromise the ability of neurons to respond to pathological changes by limiting sprouting and formation of new connections. Understanding changes in cytoskeletal dynamics and stability in development and neurodegeneration, including but not limited to regulation of MT stability, will

greatly expand our knowledge of the MT cytoskeleton in health and disease. Thymidine kinase All chemicals used were American Chemical Society quality or better, from Sigma, Invitrogen, CalBiochem, or Polysciences. Animals used include Sprague/Dawley rats (200–225 g, Harlan), male C57BL/6 (Jackson Laboratories) and TG2 KO mice (Nanda et al., 2001). Axonal transport in rat optic nerve was labeled by intravitreal injection of 35S-methionine or polyamines (3H or 14C-PUT), as described previously (Brady and Lasek, 1982). An injection-sacrifice interval (ISI) of 21 days positioned the SCa wave containing stable and labile MTs in the optic nerve. Radioactive proteins were analyzed by

SDS-PAGE and fluorography (Kirkpatrick et al., 2001). Our standard protocol for cold/Ca2+ fractionation of neuronal tubulins was used (Brady et al., 1984) (Figure 1; Supplemental Information). Following cold/Ca2+ fractionation, samples were separated on gradient gels as described and transferred to Immobilon-P membrane (Millipore). Primary antibodies include DM1A (1:20,000, Sigma) for α-tubulin, TGMO1 (1:4000) for TG2, H2 (1:50,000) (Pfister et al., 1989) for kinesin heavy chain, Tu27 (1:10,000, provided by Dr. A. Frankfurter [Caceres et al., 1984]) for β-tubulin, A2066 (1:5000, Sigma) for β-actin, and pab0022 and pab0023 (1:1200, Covalab) for SPM/SPD. For quantitative immunoblots, the secondary antibody was rat anti-mouse IgG (1:1000, Jackson) detected with 125I-Protein A and measured by PhosphorImager (Molecular Dynamics) for quantitation with ImageQuant.

The implanted microdrive assembly was produced in-house and consi

The implanted microdrive assembly was produced in-house and consisted of 8 individually drivable stereotrodes (25 μm nichrome wires, A-M Systems, Inc., Carlsborg, WA). A 2.0 mm craniotomy was prepared at −0.1 mm anterior to and 5.0 mm lateral to lambda, allowing for visualization of the transverse sinus. The electrodes were inserted 300–500 μm anterior to the transverse sinus

at a 22° angle along the mediolateral axis with tip pointed Ruxolitinib in the lateral direction. The electrodes were lowered 300 μm from the cortical surface and secured with dental cement, dental acrylic, and anchor screws. Rats were allowed 7 days to recover prior to behavioral training. At the end of the experiment, animals were given an overdose of Beuthanasia-D (100 mg/kg, i.p.), electrode tip placements were marked with a small lesion, the animals were perfused, and the brains were extracted and prepared for histology and subsequent localization of electrodes. The locations of electrode Ulixertinib cell line tips were reconstructed with a light microscope and localized in POR as defined by Burwell (2001). During recording, microdrivers were generally advanced about 1/6 turn or 55.5 μm. Total distance advanced ranged from 333 to 610.5 μm. Given this short distance and the trajectory of electrodes, we assumed that all cells recorded

from a particular stereotrode were in the layer in which the tip was histologically located. See Supplemental Experimental Procedures for details. Neuronal activity recorded from stereotrodes (McNaughton et al., 1983) was multiplied by 20 with an operational amplifier at the head stage (HST/8o50-G20-GR, Plexon, Inc., Dallas, TX). Signals were Rebamipide then passed through a differential preamplifier with a gain of 50 (PBX2/16sp-r-G50, Plexon, Inc.). Also at this stage, single-unit activity was filtered between 154–8,800 Hz and LFPs were filtered between 0.7–170 Hz (PBX2/16sp-r-G50, Plexon, Inc.). The signal was then digitized at 40 kHz for single-unit activity and 1 kHz for LFP activity and further amplified for a total gain of 10,000 (MAP system, Plexon, Inc.). Waveforms with signal-to-noise ratios greater

than ∼3:1 were extracted by real-time thresholding (Sort Client, Plexon, Inc.) and stored along with time stamps of behaviorally relevant events for offline analysis. Spikes associated with putative individual cells were isolated offline for each session using a variety of manual and partially automated techniques for classification based on waveform characteristics (Offline Sorter, Plexon, Inc). Separation of sorted spikes by at least 1 ms was verified by autocorrelograms. Sorted files were corrected for an identified issue of time alignment between spike data and field potential data using FPAlignV1 (Plexon, Inc.) (Nelson et al., 2008). Timestamps for spikes and behaviorally relevant event markers were extracted from sorted files using Neuroexplorer (NEX, Plexon, Inc.).